"Generic clomipramine 25 mg without a prescription, anxiety jackets for dogs". L. Rakus, MD Professor, Saint Louis University School of Medicine They might well be able to consciously represent the fact of being only secondclass sentient citizens mood disorder meds for kids 10mg clomipramine fast delivery, alienated postbiotic selves being used as interchangeable experimental tools bipolar depression or major depression cheap 75 mg clomipramine mastercard. How would it feel to "come to" as an advanced artificial subject depression symptoms treatment and causes purchase clomipramine 25mg, only to discover that even though you possessed a robust sense of selfhood and experienced yourself as a genuine subject depression anxiety test cheap clomipramine 75 mg overnight delivery, you were only a commodity The story of the first artificial Ego Machines, those postbiotic phenomenal selves with no civil rights and no lobby in any ethics committee, nicely illustrates how the capacity for suffering emerges along with the phenomenal Ego; suffering starts in the Ego Tunnel. It also presents a principled argument against the creation of artificial consciousness as a goal of academic research. In the same sense, all sentient beings capable of suffering should constitute a solidarity against suffering. Out of this solidarity, we should refrain from doing anything that could increase the overall amount of suffering and confusion in the universe. While all sorts of theoretical complications arise, we can agree not to gratuitously increase the overall amount of suffering in the universe-and creating Ego Machines would very likely do this right from the beginning. We could create suffering postbiotic Ego Machines before having understood which properties of our biological history, bodies, and brains are the roots of our own suffering. Preventing and minimizing suffering wherever possible also includes the ethics of risktaking: I believe we should not even risk the realization of artificial phenomenal self-models. Our attention would be better directed at understanding and neutralizing our own suffering-in philosophy as well as in the cognitive neurosciences and the field of artificial intelligence. Until we become happier beings than our ancestors were, we should refrain from any attempt to impose our mental structure on artificial carrier systems. I would argue that we should orient ourselves toward the classic philosophical goal of self-knowledge and adopt at least the minimal ethical Artificial Ego Machines 197 principle of reducing and preventing suffering, instead of recklessly embarking on a second-order evolution that could slip out of control. If there is such a thing as forbidden fruit in modern consciousness research, it is the careless multiplication of suffering through the creation of artificial Ego Tunnels without a clear grasp of the consequences. A hypothetical question suggests itself: If we could, on the other hand, increase the overall amount of pleasure and joy in the universe by flooding it with self-replicating and blissful postbiotic Ego Machines, should we do that The assumption that the first generations of artificial Ego Machines will resemble mentally retarded human infants and bring more pain, confusion, and suffering than pleasure, joy, or insight into the universe may be empirically false, for a number of reasons. Such machines conceivably might function better than we thought they would and might enjoy their existence to a much greater extent than we expected. Or, as the agents of mental evolution and the engineers of subjectivity, we could simply take care to make this assumption empirically false, constructing only those conscious systems that were either incapable of having phenomenal states such as suffering or that could enjoy existence to a higher degree than human beings do. If our new theory of consciousness eventually allowed us to turn ourselves from old-fashioned biological Ego Machines, burdened by the horrors of their biological history, into Bliss Machines- should we do it There is more to an existence worth having, or a life worth living, than subjective experience. A terminally ill cancer patient on a high dose of morphine and mood-enhancing medications can have a very positive self-image, just as drug addicts may still be able to function in their final stages. Human beings have been trying to turn themselves from Ego Machines into Bliss Machines for centuries-pharmacologically or through adopting metaphysical belief systems and mind-altering practices. In his book Anarchy, State, and Utopia, the late political philosopher Robert Nozick suggested the following thought experiment: You have the option of being hooked up to an "Experience Machine" that keeps you in a state of permanent happiness. Interestingly, Nozick found that most people would not opt to spend the rest of their lives hooked up to such a machine. The reason is that most of us do not value bliss as such, but want it grounded in truth, virtue, artistic achievement, or some sort of higher good. We want to be not deluded Bliss Machines but conscious subjects who are happy for a reason, who consciously experience existence as something worth having. We want an extraordinary insight into reality, into moral value or beauty as objective facts. He insisted that we would not want sheer happiness alone if there were no actual contact with a deeper reality-even though the subjective experience of it can in principle be simulated. That is why most of us, on second thought, would not want to flood the physical universe with blissed-out artificial Ego Machines- at least, not if these machines were in a constant state of self-deception. This leads to another issue: Everything we have learned about the transparency of phenomenal states clearly shows that "actual contact with reality" and "certainty" can be simulated too, and that nature has already done it in our brains by creating the Ego Tunnel. Just think about hallucinated agency or the phenomenon of false awakenings in dream research. I return to my earlier caveat-that we should refrain from doing anything that could increase the overall amount of suffering and confusion in the universe. Thus depression symptoms and warning signs clomipramine 75 mg mastercard, seizures arising in the first postcentral gyrus evoke somatosensory symptoms such as tingling bipolar depression 35 clomipramine 50 mg low price, pins and needles mood disorder axis 1 discount 75mg clomipramine, electrical sensations and numbness which depression symptoms marriage discount 75 mg clomipramine with amex, like their motor counterpart, may show Jacksonian progression. Similarly, seizures arising in the primary visual or auditory cortex are associated with elemental hallucinations: in the case of the primary visual cortex, flashing lights, simple shapes and patterns are commonly described, while buzzing and hissing sounds are examples of symptoms associated with epileptic foci in the primary auditory cortex (middle temporal gyrus). Olfactory and gustatory experiences are usually unpleasant (burning, metallic) or difficult for the patient to characterise, and are associated with seizures arising in the limbic system. A relatively common symptom, sometimes known as a cephalic aura, is the experience of an indescribable sensation in the head. These symptoms arise when midline basal brain structures are involved in seizure discharge. Patients may experience distortions of thought such as forced thinking, Epilepsy 311 which describes a feeling of being compelled to think about a specific topic or word; or crowding of thoughts, which describes a feeling of racing, disorganised thoughts. Subtle but disturbing changes in the quality of perception are reported, including derealisation and depersonalisation, distortions in the perception of time and changes in the significance of objects. These latter are often impossible for patients to describe but may involve a sense that a specific object in their environment seems changed and has a heightened but mysterious personal relevance. Affective symptoms are usually unpleasant and include fear, dysphoria, sadness and feelings of unworthiness or guilt. Simple partial seizures are brief, usually lasting for a few seconds only and rarely for more than 2 minutes. The most common are oro-alimentary automatisms, which include lip-smacking, chewing and swallowing movements. Repetitively picking at, or adjusting, clothing or handling objects within easy reach are also frequent (gestural automatisms). Vocal automatisms may include perseverative utterances (sometimes called epileptic pallilalia), humming, singing and laughing (gelastic seizures). The laughter in gelastic seizures typically has an unusual quality, is not infectious and seems mirthless. Wandering is common (ambulatory automatisms) and may seem semi-purposeful, as if the patient is searching for something or trying to escape, or may involve walking in circles (cursive seizures) or running. Although impairment of consciousness has conventionally been regarded as a necessary condition for the emergence of automatisms, isolated cases have been reported in which automatisms have occurred while the patient remained fully alert and responsive during clearly documented simple partial seizures (Alarcon et al. Complex partial seizures Complex partial seizures are partial seizures that involve some degree of impaired consciousness. In most complex partial seizures, however, the patient is fully aware for a few seconds at seizure onset. In these cases patients experience symptoms at the beginning of a complex seizure, known as an aura, that may include any of those associated with simple partial seizures described above. In some 20% of cases patients report no aura, indicating either that consciousness was impaired from the start or that the patient has no memory of any premonitory symptoms after the seizure. Subtle degrees of impaired consciousness may be difficult to determine, especially in the presence of dysphasia or overwhelming affective experiences and indeed this is a criticism that has been levelled at the current system of classification. Rather there is clouding of consciousness during which the patient will appear confused, disoriented and preoccupied but may interact with people and handle objects in their immediate environment, albeit in a disorganised manner and in ways that are inappropriate to the immediate social context. To the observer there is an inconspicuous and gradual transition from normal alertness to impaired responsiveness. During the phase of impaired consciousness the patient commonly engages in repetitive semi-purposeful activities known as automatisms [Classification of epilepsy syndromes (Anatomically defined localisation-related epilepsy syndromes) and Epileptic Partial seizures with secondary generalisation Approximately 60% of patients with partial seizures will experience a secondary generalised seizure at some point. Occasionally, patients have secondarily generalised seizures with almost every partial seizure. In such cases the generalised convulsion may be so dramatic that it overshadows the preceding partial seizure. For this reason, when a patient presents with apparent generalised seizures, care must always be taken to search for any evidence of a partial seizure onset: from the patients themselves, who should be questioned about aura symptoms, and from witnesses who should be asked about blank staring episodes or brief automatisms occurring before the convulsion. Generalised seizures the defining characteristic of generalised seizures is that they have no detectable focal onset: the abnormal electrical discharges that accompany clinical seizures involve the cerebral cortex bilaterally at onset. Porphyria is included as a rare but striking example of an inborn error of metabolism with important psychiatric features 3 theories of mood disorder buy clomipramine 75mg with mastercard. The chapter focuses on the clinical psychiatric manifestations of the primary endocrine disorders definition depression im kindesalter discount 75mg clomipramine mastercard, discussed together with the results of relevant research literature anxiety vitamin deficiency order clomipramine 25 mg amex. Whilst a significant majority of patients with the conditions discussed will present either to a general physician or general practitioner mood disorder ptsd discount 25 mg clomipramine amex, occasional individuals will present in such a way that psychiatric manifestations dominate the clinical picture and the endocrine disturbance may go unnoticed. Endocrine disorders can be accompanied by prominent abnormalities of the mental state, as for example in hypothyroidism and hyperthyroidism, and epochs of life such as after childbirth that are associated with rapid changes in hormonal levels appear to be associated with special liability to mental disturbance. Conversely, it is now clear that primary emotional disorders are accompanied by changes in neuroendocrine regulatory functions and may even predispose to the development of endocrine disorders such as diabetes mellitus. Historically, treatment by means of hormones has often been viewed as a possibility in psychiatry. Kraepelin (1896) once proposed that dementia praecox was basically an endocrine disorder. More recently, studies of patients treated for hypopituitarism have revealed that hormones such as growth hormone, previously thought to play a role only in childhood and adolescence, continue to be important to mental well-being and metabolic function throughout adult life. On the other hand, exogenous administration of hormones, most notably corticosteroids, may lead to the development of cognitive, psychotic or affective disorder. Recent research has continued to pursue the question of how hormones influence fundamental aspects of brain development and human behaviour during both early childhood development and later adult life. Experimental work in animals has clarified the morphological basis by which lack of thyroxine during early development impairs the maturation of behaviour (Eayers 1968). Prenatal steroid hormones have a decisive influence in animals on sexual differentiation and on a wide range of sexual and social behaviours (McCarthy 1994; Signoret & Balthazart 1994). Diabetes mellitus Diabetes mellitus is a group of metabolic diseases characterized by hyperglycaemia resulting from defects in insulin production, insulin action or both. Recent consensus opinion has advised a change to an aetiopathologically based classificatory system (Expert Committee on the Diagnosis and Classification of Diabetes Mellitus 2003), with the vast majority of cases fitting into one of two broad categories. In type 1 diabetes the associated abnormalities of protein, carbohydrate and fat metabolism are the result of insufficient insulin action on peripheral target tissues as a result of reduced insulin secretion, whereas in type 2 diabetes these metabolic abnormalities are the result of diminished tissue response to insulin with or without an associated deficiency in insulin secretion. Type 1 diabetes is associated with pancreatic islet -cell loss that results in proneness to ketosis. Most cases typically present by age 20 and appear to be the result of an autoimmune reaction, possibly triggered by an environmental insult, on a background of raised genetic susceptibility. Type 2 diabetes likely results from a combination of inadequate insulin secretion and peripheral insulin resistance. Insulin levels may be higher than seen in normals but are insufficient to overcome resistance in liver, muscle and adipose tissue. It is well established that genetic mechanisms contribute to the development of both forms of the disease, and interesting progress has been made in relation to type 1 diabetes (Bennett et al. The insulin gene is flanked upstream by multiple repeats of a 14-bp sequence, variations in length of the sequence correlating with disease susceptibility, perhaps through a direct effect on transcription of the insulin gene. Textbooks of medicine should be consulted for the general clinical associations of the disorder and the principles of management by diet, insulin and oral hypoglycaemic agents. Psychiatric disorders, particularly emotional disorders, are more prevalent in the diabetic population, whilst the development of depression in diabetic patients is associated with poorer glycaemic control, higher prevalence of multiple diabetic complications and greater functional impairment. Furthermore, mood disorder appears to be an independent risk factor for the development of type 2 diabetes. A number of medications commonly used by psychiatrists, including all the antipsychotic drugs, are associated with an increased incidence of diabetes. It is therefore important that all practising psychiatrists should be familiar with current diagnostic criteria for diabetes mellitus, enabling any patient developing diabetes to be rapidly identified and referred for appropriate treatment. Failure to do so will unnecessarily expose patients seen by psychiatrists to increased risk of developing cardiovascular disease and diabetic microvascular complications. These issues are discussed below, along with the question of brain damage in diabetic patients. When evidence of brain damage emerges this may be attributable to episodes of hypoglycaemia or diabetic coma or, alternatively, to the high incidence of atherosclerosis that exists in patients with diabetes. The picture of diabetic coma, and certain common neurological complications, are also briefly described. In several ways the situation imposed by diabetes is unusual in comparison with other chronic diseases. The neuropsychological deficit the amnesic syndrome was for many years regarded as reflecting a failure of consolidation of new experience depression definition deutsch buy clomipramine 75 mg low price. Thus while the immediate memory span is normal depression symptoms for 13 year olds buy 75mg clomipramine with visa, and early memories may remain substantially intact depression hashtags clomipramine 75mg mastercard, current experience cannot gain proper access to the secondary memory (Milner 1966) mood disorder fellowship purchase 25mg clomipramine free shipping. However, a simple consolidation hypothesis is hard pressed to explain why some forms of cueing can improve performance, or why patients can achieve better results on recognition tests than when tested by free recall. Moreover, if consolidation were the explanation of an extensive retrograde amnesia, where it occurs, this would imply that the process of physiological consolidation lasts for years, even decades. Butters and colleagues stressed the role of deficient semantic encoding of information in leading to the poor performance of amnesic subjects (Butters & Cermak 1980). This, it was argued, might account in considerable degree for their failure to store material adequately. More recently, this theory has evolved into a more generalised notion of a deficit in binding complex associations (Mayes & Downes 1997) or in binding the relations between items (Cohen et al. It was found that, after learning has been acquired, many amnesic patients show a normal rate of forgetting, at least on recognition memory tests (Huppert & Piercy 1978b; Kopelman & Stanhope 1997), but there is some evidence that patients with medial temporal lobe pathology might forget at an accelerated rate, even after learning has been acquired (Huppert & Piercy 1979; Parkin & Leng 1988). However, various studies have failed to demonstrate this (Kopelman 1985a; McKee & Squire 1992), although there is some evidence that, over and above their initial acquisition or learning deficit, amnesic patients show accelerated forgetting when tested on recall (as opposed to recognition) memory tasks over a period of minutes (Kopelman & Stanhope 1997; Green & Kopelman 2002). Warrington and Weiskrantz (1968) postulated that amnesic patients were unable to suppress inappropriate responses during recall and recognition memory tasks. They noted that such patients sometimes respond erroneously with what had been the correct responses to previous test items, and that the provision of retrieval cues can improve their performance. On the other hand, it was later found that healthy subjects exhibited these phenomena when given memory tests at relatively long delay intervals, suggesting that they were a consequence of poor memory rather than its cause (Mayes & Meudell 1981). Amnesic syndrome or syndromes In the past, there was an extensive debate concerning whether there was a differential pattern of amnesic deficit in comparing patients with diencephalic and those with medial temporal lobe pathologies. In contrast, others argued for differential patterns of memory deficits on the basis of findings with respect to measures of forgetting rates or contextual (temporal and spatial) memory (Huppert & Piercy 1979; Parkin 1987). In general, although there may be subtle differences in contextual memory, these differential patterns have not been corroborated (Kopelman 2002). Moreover, although a broad distinction between executive processes and encoding/retrieval mechanisms remains valid, there is also considerable overlap with the effects of large (particularly bilateral) frontal lesions, and a review of the latter showed that virtually all studies have reported impairments in recall (and often recognition) memory (Wheeler et al. More recently, differences have been sought between the effects of damage to hippocampal and parahippocampal (particularly perirhinal) structures. Aggleton and Shaw (1996) argued that patients with pathology confined to the hippocampi showed impairments on verbal and visual recall but not recognition memory, whereas damage to parahippocampal structures was required to produce an impairment in familiarity-based or recognition memory. There were problems with the meta-analysis on which this hypothesis was based, but supportive evidence has been obtained in a number of investigations, notably by Holdstock et al. On the other hand, others have argued that when appropriate experimental controls are introduced, patients with pathology confined to the hippocampi (as well as other amnesic patients) showed proportionate impairments on both recall and recognition memory procedures (Reed & Squire 1997; Manns et al. It is difficult, in contrast, to provide an explanation for the very long retrograde amnesias that may extend for years or decades before the onset of an amnesic syndrome. A number of authors have previously made this distinction between the different characteristics of short- and long-term retrograde amnesia (Symonds 1966; Kapur 1999). Neuropsychological studies have attempted to map the pattern of retrograde amnesia in amnesic disorders of various aetiologies. In a pioneering study, Sanders and Warrington (1971) employed a test requiring the recall and recognition of famous faces and a standardised questionnaire about public events from different time periods, finding a retrograde amnesia extending back many decades in a small group of amnesic patients. Subsequently, measures of personal or autobiographical memory were introduced (Zola-Morgan et al. Interesting hemispheric differences have been reported in memory-disordered patients. This latter pattern has usually been attributed to the problems in retrieving the visual imagery associated with autobiographical memories. These observations have implications for the interpretation of a lengthy retrograde amnesia. |